Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Past Simple
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Passive and Active
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Low-Level Lengthening
المؤلف: APRIL McMAHON
المصدر: LEXICAL PHONOLOGY AND THE HISTORY OF ENGLISH
الجزء والصفحة: P182-C4
2024-12-18
89
Low-Level Lengthening
There seems to be a consensus of opinion among phoneticians that vowels lengthen progressively according to the hierarchy of following consonants shown in (1), with even greater duration pre-pausally (House and Fairbanks 1953, Peterson and Lehiste 1960, House 1961, Delattre 1962, Chen 1970).
(1)
This `voicing effect' process certainly seems to apply in most varieties of English; it may not be characteristic of Northern English dialects (Roger Lass, personal communication), although again experimental evidence is lacking here. Peterson and Lehiste (1960) give measurements for American English which show that, while preceding consonants appear to have a negligible effect on the duration of following vowels, average vowel durations can more than double between the shortest context ([-p]) and the longest ([-z] for short vowels, [-Ʒ] for long). Wiik (1965) gives similar results for RP. The most salient effect seems to be that of voicing: the vowel durations of Peterson and Lehiste's informants before voiced as opposed to voiceless consonants, in otherwise identical environments, formed a ratio of 3:2.
There are likely to be universal phonetic factors underlying the variable lengthening effect of following consonants (Zimmerman and Sapon 1958), perhaps reflecting the operation of a type of compensatory lengthening: if roughly the same time is allotted to each VC sequence in an utterance, and voiceless consonants are longer than voiced, vowels before voiced consonants may lengthen to maintain a quasi-constant duration for the VC sequence. Whatever the physiological or articulatory motivation for this lengthening process, evidence from Gandour, Weinberg and Rutkowski (1980) indicates that it operates as a language specific phonological rule of English. Gandour et al. argue that, if the voicing effect is purely physiological and due to laryngeal adjustment of some kind, it would not be expected in oesophageal speech. They tested three normal adult males and three laryngectomized patients, all in their fifties. All had hearing in the normal range for their age-group, and none had speech impediments. The laryngectomees were recommended by speech pathologists as having fluent, highly intelligible oesophageal speech. Gandour et al. found that the duration of vowels before voiced consonants was significantly longer than before voiceless consonants for both groups, at the level of p 0.01. There was no significant difference across speaker groups for duration before voiceless consonants, but vowels were significantly longer in voiced contexts for the oesophageal as opposed to the normal speakers. This extra length might be attributed to the slower average speaking speed for the laryngectomized patients, at 2.01 as opposed to 2.97 syllables per second; but if this were the only determining factor, we would expect the relative length difference to carry over to voiceless contexts. It would therefore appear that oesophageal speakers are signalling the voicing effect lengthening, and perhaps even exaggerating it, indicating that `natural phonetic tendencies have apparently been expanded into a phonological rule of the grammar' (Gandour, Weinberg and Rutkowski 1980: 150). I shall call this lengthening process, which is dependent on the voicing effect, Low-Level Lengthening (LLL), and will argue below that it applies postlexically, while SVLR is a lexical phonological rule in Scots and SSE.