Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Simple
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Passive and Active
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Semiotics
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Teaching Methods
Teaching Strategies
Assessment
ORDEROFACQUISITION
المؤلف:
John Field
المصدر:
Psycholinguistics
الجزء والصفحة:
P194
2025-09-21
39
ORDEROFACQUISITION
A fixed order in which an infant acquires the syntactic, morphological or phonological features of its target language. If a natural order of this kind exists, it might provide evidence in support of the nativist notion that human beings are innately endowed with a Universal Grammar which determines the way in which their first language is acquired.
Early evidence from so-called morpheme studies suggested that certain inflections of English were indeed acquired in a fixed order. The age and rate at which they were acquired by the infant might vary; but the order remained relatively constant. The first inflection acquired (between 19 and 28 months) is usually-ing as in Mommy driving. It is followed by the-s plural at 27 to 33 months. Next come the possessive and the irregular Past Simple. The features selected for these studies included function words as well as inflections. Some of the functors (e.g. prepositions) were semantically quite transparent and shown to be acquired early; others (e.g. the articles) were more complex and delayed.
The results were not as conclusive as is sometimes suggested: they showed a considerable time overlap between certain features. In addition, there are a number of serious problems with this kind of study– not least the difficulty of establishing at what point a child can be treated as having ‘acquired’ a particular form. The usual yardstick is 90 per cent accuracy of use, but this ignores the fact that the child might have mastered the form receptively long before producing it. Furthermore, many inflections first occur attached to a limited set of words– suggesting that these particular items may have been acquired as a single unit. A further problem in determining ‘acquisition’ is that, after apparently mastering a form, the child may show signs of U- shaped development where the correct form is replaced by an erroneous one. Finally, considerations of meaning as well as form may be involved: -ing tends to appear first on durative verbs (e.g. driving) and-ed on verbs of rapid action (dropped). This suggests that these forms are first acquired as unanalysed vocabulary items.
Later research has considered semantic as well as formal criteria. It suggested that deixis (this, that, here, there) emerges early, though infants have difficulty in adjusting to the viewpoint of their co-locutor. There seems to be considerable variation in the age at which definite and indefinite articles are used consistently, though the distinction between commonand proper nouns (That’s Dax vs That’s a dax) is grasped quite quickly. It has been noted that subject pronouns are often absent from early utterances. I and it tend to appear before you, singular forms before plural and masculine before feminine.
If there is indeed a consistent order of acquisition for certain inflections and syntactic features, the reasons may lie within the language being acquired rather than in the projections of a universal grammar. It appears that the frequency with which an item occurs in adult speech does not correlate with the speed at which an infant acquires it. Instead, cross-lingual studies have suggested that the order of acquisition may reflect the relative difficulty of a feature within a particular language. Thus the plural is acquired quite early in English but later in Arabic (which has a dual form as well as a plural and distinguishes gender). Similarly, gender emerges early in English, where it is related to natural gender and chiefly affects the he/she contrast; but much later in Fulani, a West African language which is remarkable for having 12 genders.
Variations may also reflect the relative importance of a feature. Children acquiring highly inflected languages (Russian, Hungarian) show an early sensitivity to inflections. There is evidence of rapid and accurate mastery of inflections among children acquiring Greenlandic Eskimo, Japanese, Turkish and German. Similarly, in languages where word order is important, children seem to recognise the importance of order very early.
Variation in order of acquisition between languages is neatly accounted for in Chomskyan theory by suggesting that the infant has to set certain parameters in relation to the language it is acquiring (for example, it has to determine whether the language licenses the omission of subject pronouns or not). Order of acquisition thus reflects the relative difficulty of setting different parameters.
One theory in the Chomskyan tradition (Radford, 1990) suggests that infants start out with an innate grammar that is based upon a reduced system, lacking inflections, determiners and complementiser phrases (I know that he is innocent). The order in which features of language are acquired is determined by the gradual development of these tree branches within the child’s grammar.
See also: Language acquisition: stages, Syntactic development
Further reading: Aitchison (1998: Chap. 4); Fletcher and Garman (1986: 307–447); Foster-Cohen (1999: Chaps 6–7); Slobin (1982)
الاكثر قراءة في Linguistics fields
اخر الاخبار
اخبار العتبة العباسية المقدسة

الآخبار الصحية
